Democratic Values in a Globalizing World:
A Multilevel Analysis of Geographic
Contexts.[1]
Department of Geography
Campus
Boulder, Co.
80309-0487
Email:
johno@colorado.edu
Abstract
Geographers contend that regional and national contexts
are important mediating and controlling influences on globalization
processes. However, to reach this
conclusion, geographers have been forced to engage in rather convoluted
statistical manipulations to try to isolate the so-called "geographic
factor". Recent developments in
multilevel statistical modeling offer a more precise and suitable methodology
for examination of contextual factors in political behavior if the data have
been collected in a hierarchical manner with respondents grouped into
lower-level and higher-level districts.
The World Values Survey data (collected in three waves from 1980 to
1997) for 65 countries are ideally suited to examination of the hypothesis that
democratic beliefs and practices are globalizing. Using three key predictors (trust in fellow
citizens, political interest, and volunteerism) for the sample of 91,160
respondents, it is evident that regional (for the 550 regions) and country
settings (between 55 and 65 countries) are important predictors of political
behavior, on the order of about 10% and 20% respectively. Respondent characteristics account for about
70% of the variance explained. Ideology
is far more significant than many of the usual demographic characteristics in
explaining political behavior cross-nationally.
Dramatic differences between established and new democracies clarify the
political globalization process and global regions (
Using data from 1946-1994 and a measure of democracy
based on political authority characteristics,
democratization has proceeded in regular spatial and temporal diffusion
patterns and distinct regional trends can also be observed (O’Loughlin et al.,
1998). Unlike the temporal diffusion
suggested by
The reversal to authoritarianism
anticipated by
Recognition of
the importance of location as a factor in political developments continues to
grow. First, being part of the same
region or sharing a border with a state undergoing profound political change
increases the chances of political transformations in neighboring countries (Kopstein and O’Reilly, 2000; O’Loughlin, 2002). Examples from sub-Saharan
A third trend parallels the methodological gap in political science
between the comparativists, who tend to study one
polity or examine a small set of countries, and the macro-structuralists,
who engage in cross-sectional analysis of the countries that constitute the
world system; their separate approaches do not really encourage a rapprochement
on the basis of the shared interest in regional affairs. While political scientists increasingly
accept and use the geographic techniques of spatial analysis, there is still a
significant knowledge lag between the disciplines about concepts of space and
place. Most geographers adhere strongly
to notions of “place” as complex areal units that are
shaped by human behavior, beliefs and values over a long period of time (
For the past two decades, geographers have manipulated aggregate data
to demonstrate the small, but statistical significant, effects of the context
in which political acts takes place. In
almost all instances, empirical analysis examines voting statistics, though
spatial analyses of other kinds of political data such as international
conflict behavior follow the same general modeling procedures (O’Loughlin,
1986; Kirby and Ward, 1987; O’Loughlin and Anselin, 1991). Important extensions in the spatial analysis
tool set in the 1980s and the integration of these methods with GIS (Geographic
Information Science) visualization techniques allowed geographers to show how
the usual regression equations of aggregate data were probably incorrect –
biased and inefficient estimators and significant clustering (dependence) in
the residuals – and that these kinds of aggregate data required the application
of the specialized tools of spatial analysis.
MacAllister (1987) and King (1996, 1997)
argued that adding the right kind of predictor variables, fitting the right
kind of model (perhaps log-linear), avoiding the ecological fallacy, collecting
the right kind of information to answer a specific question, or analyzing at
the right scale (more localized analyses) will see the evaporation of the
“geographic factor”. Geographers,
especially
In recent years, geographers have increasingly turned to survey data
for individuals to report the existence of small but significant contextual
effects in political behavior and attitudes.
Pattie and
Like other social scientists, geographers are beginning to move from
a “dummy variable” to a multilevel modeling approach for individual level data. If more than a few regions exist, use of the
dummy variables becomes cumbersome and worse yet, the
nuances of place are poorly captured.
Alternatively, one could fit the same model for different scales
(individual, regional, national) if the requisite data were available but
cross-scale effects are hidden in this approach. In multilevel modeling, on the other hand, a
single regression model handles the micro-scale (individuals), the meso-scale (regions or towns) and the macro-scale (states
or countries). Moreover, multilevel
models allow relationships to vary according to geographic context, thus
speaking to the heart of the division that separates geographers and political
scientists. Widely used in public health
studies and educational research to determine the separate and interactive
effects of the characteristics of people and their contextual settings
(communities and schools), multilevel modeling is rapidly growing in use in all
the other social sciences. With both the availability of specialized computer
software and the growing recognition that many models are too general (fit for
aggregate data across the varied contexts within a study site) or too specific
(targeted to the characteristics of individuals with no attention to their
environments), multilevel modeling can be expected to gain more adherents
quickly.
Jones and Duncan (1996) provide a list of contextual analyses that
can be accommodated within the multilevel framework. Consider the topic of this chapter, the
explanation of the variation in democratic beliefs, measured by answers to the
question of whether the respondent thinks that democracy is the best political
system. First, multilevel modeling can
detect and measure contextual differences by considering simultaneously
personal attributes (the micro-scale) and the macro-scale of the country where the respondents
live. Second, place heterogeneity can be
measured so that we can see how different factors are related to democratic
beliefs across the countries. Third,
perhaps the greatest potential of multilevel modeling is to take the
interaction of place and individual socio-demographic attributes into
account. A respondent may answer quite
differently about democracy depending on the ideology or government style of
the country in which he/she is a citizen.
Whether through intimidation, pressure or conversion, ethnic or class determinants
of political attitudes can take on different dimensions in different
countries. Fourth, multilevel modeling
does not assume that all voters of a particular class or other socio-economic
group behave in the same manner.
Individual heterogeneity is also determined and measured. Fifth, panel data of a longitudinal nature
(same respondents at different times) can be modeled in a multilevel fashion so
that each wave can be considered as a separate scale and the effects of
changing context over time also measured.
Sixth, since it is probable based on previous research that voters have
multiple contextual influences (home, work, church, neighborhood),
multilevel modeling allows the measurement of these separate environments. While all of these different modeling
strategies can be accomplished using familiar multiple regression procedures,
the adaptations to achieve them are cumbersome and require the use of multiple
dummy predictors and a large number of terms in the equation.
Democracy and Political Values in a
Globalized World
A truism highlights two contradictory trends that characterize contemporary citizens. Across the globe, growing numbers of people express support for democracy as a value system while in the longest-established democracies, more citizens than ever are dissatisfied with democratic procedures and especially, with the performance of governmental regimes at all levels (Nye, Zellikov and King, 1997; Dalton, 1999; Pharr and Putnam, 2000). Even in the new democracies (established after 1989), citizens are increasingly critical of their governments’ performances and while not taking to the streets to protest their dissatisfaction, they nevertheless are becoming “critical citizens” like their Western counterparts (Norris, 1999b). The most vulnerable democracies (candidates for a reversal to authoritarianism after Huntington’s Third Wave) are those that are labeled “partly free” in the Freedom House lexicon, as they seem to be plagued with ethnic tensions, regional and religious polarizations, administrative corruption, controlled elections and weak mass media, partly-functioning or brow-beaten legislatures, and un-consolidated party systems. These “democracies” are in danger of not consolidating the gains of the 1980s and 1990s.
In a major project to ascertain the state of democratic values world-wide, Norris and her colleagues derive three main conclusions from numerous studies of a wide range of democracies – a) political support is not one type and needs to be disaggregated into its different components, b) growing numbers of citizens are critical of government performance in rich countries and established democracies, and c) there is a growing tension everywhere between democratic ideals and reality. The worry for promoters of democracy is that if support for democratic institutions is falling, then support for democratic values can also be jeopardized (Norris, 1999c, 26). The terms “critical citizens” or “dissatisfied democrats” well describe the current state of play. Explaining the variation in institutional confidence is not simple, with only a few variables (at the individual level) significantly related to it. Conventional democratic participation (voting, volunteerism, etc), political attitudes, and national context explain some of the variation but there is only a weak correlation between institutional confidence and protest potential (Inglehart, 1999).
The national context variable appears consistently as an important factor in setting the nature of democratic values leading Inglehart (1999, 266) to conclude that “we strongly suspect that a supportive political culture is necessary for democratic consolidation but the exact weight to be given to it is a matter of debate.” Inglehart and his associates have tracked the rise of a change in social values that they call “post materialism” in many countries for over 30 years. Though there is a correlation between materialist – post-materialist value ratios and economic fluctuations, the noteworthy trend is an inexorable rise in post-materialism in rich countries, a strengthening of materialist values in poor countries, and a generational change toward post-materialist values. As the public favors more public voice in governmental decisions (part of a democratic culture’s expectations) due to rising levels of education, democratic institutions must adapt to these expectations or come under increased questioning by citizens. The long-term prospect anticipates mass publics becoming increasingly supportive of democratic institutions as more countries become richer – though established democracies will have to be careful in how they respond to their citizenry (Abramson and Inglehart, 1995; Inglehart, 1997, 1999).
Generalized conclusions about democratic values, institutional performance and post-materialist developments are drawn from cross-national surveys. In order to make comparative statements, it is first important to establish equivalence in the concepts, terms, phenomena and definitions used in the different national contexts. In designing the “political values” project, Inglehart (1997, 63) picked a general strategy that designed broadly relevant questions in order to examine to what extent their structure, connotations, demographic correlates, and constraints are cross-culturally similar. Factor analysis of the responses to the same survey questions across countries shows that key indicators line up in the same manner in different national settings (Klingenmann, 1999), allowing Inglehart to develop his 12-item post-materialist index. However, there is no insistence on forcing similar interpretations onto different settings; interpreting results still requires an awareness of the differences in meaning across cultures. For example, there is a noticeable difference in the meaning of post-materialism between Western and the former socialist countries and between industrial and low-income countries. As van Deth (1997, 4) notes, “comparative research must start from the axiom that even similar phenomena are never identical. The question is whether we can restrict the differences between the phenomena to intrinsic, non-relational properties irrelevant to the goals of our research.”
The World Values survey has a twenty-year history, though its antecedents stretch back to the early years of the Eurobarometer surveys in the European Union states. Three waves of surveys have now been completed (1981-84, 1990-93, and 1995-1997) and the temporal and spatial coverage is very impressive, covering 45% of the world’s population. The survey relies on national teams but the nature of the voluntaristic group enterprise means that not all survey instruments are identical, not all sampling procedures are the same, and not all surveys are temporally coincident (Inglehart et al, 2000). Despite these caveats, this enormous data set constitutes the best information for cross-national examination of political, social, cultural, religious, and ideological values and with its ancillary socio-demographic data, allows a check on assumptions about the spread of democratic values, the arrival of global norms to new settings, the regional concentration of cultural affiliations and traditions, the diffusion of post-materialism, the extent of critical citizenship and number of dissatisfied democrats, and the depth of democratic feelings in democracies, old and new, established and transitional. It is the data set that I choose for the purposes of teasing out the extent to which national and regional contexts play a role in these global developments. Global trends might be sweeping aside traditional regional and national value systems producing an “international political culture” or conversely, local attachments and historical memories and legacies continue to shape external values to produce a world of cultural mosaics and democratic diversity within political globalization.
The Multilevel Modeling Procedure
As is ordinary least squares regression, multilevel models operate on the principle that each response is a result of systematic components and fluctuations across the levels. In the language of regression, each model thus has fixed and random parameters (Goldstein, 1995; Hox, 1995). Critical to the application of multilevel models is a hierarchical data structure. In this chapter, survey respondents are the first level, embedded in regions at the second level and these regions in turn are nested in countries at the third level; this is the structure of the World Values Survey data. Minimum requirements of cases apply to each level and a rule of thumb suggests that there should be at least 15-20 cases per unit at the next highest level. The selection of the World Values data for this study generated 91,196 cases at the first level, 550 regions at the second level and 65 countries at the third level (though the exact number of cases in each model depends on the mix of independent and dependent variables in the equation and their respective missing data values). The most common usage of multilevel models has been in educational settings (e.g., how much of a pupil’s test score can be attributed to the pupil’s abilities and how much to the school environment?), public health (e.g., how much of a person’s lifestyle choices such as cigarette smoking can be attributed to the person’s social status and how much to environmental influences in the form of peer pressure and community practices?) and voting behavior (e.g., what is the relative importance of a voter’s socio-demographic characteristics and his/her community setting in determining voting choice?).
In regression, a key assumption is independence of the
observations. Fitting an OLS model for individual data in the presence of
autocorrelation within the groups violates one of the assumptions of regression
– independence of the observations. If the context in which the respondents live
exercises a significant effect on their attitudes, this assumption is
violated. Ignoring clustering of
individuals will generally cause standard errors of regression coefficients to
be underestimated (elevating the significance of the predictors) when the
variation could be ascribed to chance but in fact, is based on the groups (Kreft and de Leeuw, 1998; Rasbash et al., 2000). Conflating the levels of analysis is also
common so that inferences derived from one level are often applied to another,
termed the ecological fallacy.
Specifically ordering the data in a hierarchical mode allows attention
to the interactive effects between levels and promotes a clear understanding of
where (which level) and how effects are occurring. In multilevel analysis, the groups (countries
in my case) at the second level are treated as a random sample of the
population of groups.
Building a multilevel model is an
iterative process adding more explanatory variables onto the first model. Typically, modeling begins by allocating
variance to each of the levels, a purely random effects model. If we adopted the usual regression approach,
we would fit an explanatory model for each country (with dependent and
independent variables for each respondent), thus yielding 65 separate
equations. In this procedure, we assume
that each country has different intercept coefficients, β0j and different slope coefficients, β1j. The random errors εij for each country
are assumed to have a mean of zero and a variance of σj2. In the multilevel model, however, we
assume that the variance is the same in all countries and specify this common
error variance as σ2.
The slope and intercept coefficients are
assumed to vary across the countries.
Stating the assumption in verbal terms, for respondents with the same
class status, a country with a high value of the intercept is predicted to
produce higher democratic values (say, on the question of “do you trust your
fellow citizens?”) than countries with a low value of the intercept. Further, differences in the slope coefficient
for the independent predictors are interpreted to mean that not all countries
have the same relationship between the outcome (political value) and predictor
variables. Some countries, perhaps long
term stable democracies, may have a strong effect while others, perhaps former
Communist states in
After
demonstrating these varying effects, the next step in the multilevel modeling
procedure is to introduce explanatory variables at the second level, countries
in this example. The multilevel modeling
approach has the strong advantage that it allows us to see if political values
are significantly affected by country residence and citizenship – or the
converse, whether a person’s characteristics (education, age, gender, etc) are
all that we need to know in order to account for the variance in political
beliefs. If countries matter, serious
consideration must then be given to local factors in accounting for the
institution and consolidation of democracy; if countries are unimportant, then
we can anticipate a global spread of democratic beliefs (and practices) as
income and educational gains diffuse across the globe and international norms
of democracy are adopted without respect to country setting. In this chapter, a dummy variable that
distinguishes between stable democracies (over 20 years democratic) and other
countries provided the only useful distinction at this stage of the analysis.
The
main aim of multilevel modeling is to separate and measure fixed and random
effects. Starting from a simple bivariate regression equation, we can extend it to a
multilevel model. In the bivariate regression equation, the subscript i refers to the
individual respondent:
yi = β0χ 0
+ β1χ1i +
εi (1).
This simple model at the
individual-level is referred to as the micro-model and can be fitted for all
countries in the sample with yi denoting a
respondent’s score on a political trust variable, χ1i denoting age (a
typical independent predictor), εi the
individual-level residuals, and χ0 is the
constant. The two fixed parameters, β0
(intercept) and β1
(slope
showing the change in political trust with increasing age) are interpreted as
usual. For multilevel modeling, the random
effects captured in the εi are highly
important and rather than simply allocating them to the “unexplained variance”
category, their values can be used in further modeling. A more realistic model that does not simply
assume the error terms have a mean of zero and a constant variability can be
developed by allowing the political trust measure to vary from country to
country, at the higher-level (second level) of a macro-model. Formally,
β0j = β0
+ μj (2)
In equation (2) for the second level, β0j , the average value of the social trust variable in country j, is a function of the country-wide average, β0, as well as a varying difference μj between each country and the overall countries average. We can combine equations (1) and (2), the micro- and macro-models to make a two-level mixed model:
yij = β0χ0 + β1χ1ij + (μj + εij) (3)
with the subscript ij denoting respondent i in country j, and the terms inside the parentheses indicate the random part of
the model. We make the standard
assumption that they follow a normal distribution so that it is sufficient to
estimate their variances, σ2μ
and σ2ε .
In this model, the same age-political trust holds for each country (same
slope) but the intercept (β0
+ μj) varies according to country. A further
extension of the multilevel model allows the slopes to vary between countries
so that the age-trust relationship can take on different forms according to the
national context. Another two-level
model is needed for this relationship of the form:
βij = β1 + Γj (4)
where the country
slope term is a global average plus the variation from country to country, Γj. We can now combine equations
(1), (2) and (4) generates the full fixed and random effects model of the form:
yij = β0χ0
+ β1χ1ij + (Γjχ1ij
+ μj + εij) (5)
in which the
slopes and intercepts are allowed to vary.
In equation (5), six values have to be estimated - the two fixed
coefficients, three variances/covariances at level 2,
and one variance at level 1. In this
paper, I estimate the values for this general class of model with fixed and
random coefficients with the random terms allowed to vary at any level. If the variances are small, then political
trust is a function only of age with no contextual effects. However, anticipating the results presented
below, the variances are of a significant size and the conclusion must be that
a combination of fixed parameters (reflecting the socio-demographic
characteristics of the individuals) and random effects (the contextual and individual
variances) is needed for an adequate explanation of the variation of political
values across the world.
In building a multilevel
model, the usual procedure is to start with a variance components model to
determine if there is any variance in the second and higher levels, in addition
to the variance at the first level (the individual voters). Should there be no evidence of higher-level
variance, a simple regression model is appropriate since there is no geographic
variance visible. In the variance components
model, only random parameters are present.
Depending on the nature of the information available and the quest for
either model building or model testing, fixed parameters are added in a
stepwise manner or all independent predictors are entered simultaneously. The estimation procedure typically uses an
IGLS (Iterative Generalized Least Squares), a maximum likelihood estimation
procedure. In the case where the
intra-unit correlations are small, there is reasonably good agreement between
the multilevel estimates and the OLS ones (Goldstein, 1995, 25). IGLS starts from initial OLS estimates for
the fixed coefficients and builds upon the residuals from the OLS model. At each iteration,
the weights are adjusted and changed and the residuals re-used in the next
iteration. All models were estimated
using the specialized multilevel modeling software, MlwiN
(Rasbash et al., 2000).
Data for
Examining Civic Democracy
The number of countries sampled in the World Values
survey varies from wave to wave and in order to have the most complete global
coverage possible while still maintaining temporal consistency, I opted to
combine information from the last two waves (1990-93 and 1995-1997) in this
study. Only the most recent information
was taken for each country. For example, since the same questions were asked
for a
Choosing
from among the myriad of questions asked in the cross-national survey, I probed
three different elements of democracy.
Rather than focusing on formal democratic institutions and norms such as
elections, I opted to examine the “civil basis of democracy”; what extent are people connected to
fellow citizens, are actively engaged in their communities through volunteerism
and are interested in politics? I was
motivated by Robert Putnam’s (2000) recent work on the
Trust in
fellow citizens does not correlate well with trust in democratic institutions (
Because
yearly Eurobarometer data from the early 1970s and a recent surge in research
in the former Communist countries and in the
The
first dependent variable measured social trust and was derived from another of
the World Values survey questions (http://wvs.isr.umich.edu/index.html). To
construct the binary dependent trust variable, I added the “you have to be very
careful” and the “don’t knows” together.
The average score for all sample countries was 26.3% trust with a range
from 2.8% in
The second variable, volunteerism,
varies dramatically across societies as a result of traditional attitudes,
religious practices, cultural expectations, and recent experiences of
authoritarianism. The American
democratic model from the early nineteenth-century has promoted civic
engagement in the form of grassroots activism and a strong non-state sector as
antidotes to overweening regime power.
Since Putnam’s (1993) book on Italian civil society, investigation of
the nature, extent, depth and developments in civic engagement has been carried
out in many of the new democracies, especially in
With the rise in “critical
citizenship” in rich countries, interest in formal politics is waning as
attention shifts to more local, grassroots civic activism. In the former Communist countries, older
people have often experienced four types of regimes, including Nazism and
Communism, and democracy is now evaluated in comparison to these discredited
political alternatives (Mishler and Rose, 2001). Alienation from the political system is
growing and electoral turnout rates are falling as many regimes are blamed for
declines in living standards. Mishler and Rose
therefore conclude that support for the new democracies is relative and
contingent. If one only uses electoral
turnout rates as the indicator of political interest, it appears as if the
traditional democracies are unable to engage their citizens in politics and
that the newer democracies are repeating the experience of a long slow steady
decline in electoral participation. Held
(1993) in Prospects for Democracy asks if democratization is essentially
a Western project or something of wider universal appeal. As Western democratic societies lose interest
in politics, will other regions follow suit?
Democracy relies on an active, informed citizenry for its successful
operation. If political interest
continues to decline as it has in
The organizations for the
volunteerism index are church or religious organization; sport or recreation
organization; art, music or educational organization; labor union; political
party; environmental organization; professional association; charitable
organization; or any other voluntary organization. To construct the binary dependent variable, I
counted any active membership as volunteerism (respondent answered yes to any
of the organizational membership questions).
In summary, 61.8% of all respondents stated that they volunteered in
some organization, with a range across countries from a low of 25.1% in
The final dependent variable examined
here is political interest. It is
derived from the answers to the question:
“Please say, for each of the following, how important is it in your
life” Options for responses were very important, rather important, not very
important, not at all important, and don’t know. Among the list of interests was “politics”. In order to form the dummy dependent
variable, I combined “very important” and “rather important” as political
interested and “not very important”, “not at all important” and “don’t know” as
not interested in politics. Figure 3
shows the average country value for this indicator, with values less than 2.5
indicating high interest. Highest
values are seen in
All of the surveys in the World Values project were carried out through face-to-face interviews, with a sampling universe consisting of all adult citizens, aged 18 and over, in the participating countries. In the usual sampling design, a multi-stage random selection of sampling points within each country was developed with a number of points being drawn from all administrative regional units after stratification by region and degree of urbanization. In each sampling point, a starting point address was drawn at random. Further addresses were selected by random route procedures. Some weighting was initiated to account for expected response rates by region, ethnic group and urbanization (see the World Values website http://wvs.isr.umich.edu/index.html for details).
The
Political Geography of World Democratic Values
The analyses of the three dependent variables (social trust, volunteerism and political interest) will be reported separately, though it should be remembered that democratic values are rarely so finitely defined and disconnected. For most individuals and for most societies, scores on the separate democratic values are consistent and overlapping. I considered the option of constructing dimensions of democratic values using the individual variables and analyzing the resulting principal components scores. While attractive in principle, the interpretation of the multilevel modeling results of these aggregated and complex scores would be difficult. The option of selecting individual scores carefully to reflect some of the range of democratic values was followed instead.
The variance components model, the usual starting point for multilevel modeling, is presented in Table 1. The fixed parameter for “thin trust” (trust in people) refers to the intercept value (-1.124) and reflects the log-odds of trusting fellow citizens. When transformed, the odds are .896 of an individual trusting his/her fellow citizens (“Most people can be trusted a lot”). All of the random effects for the social trust model are significant indicating that the three levels (individual, region and country) must be considered in the model. As might be expected, the variance at the level of the individual is most prominent but that for the country level (third level) is also very important while the regional factor is less so. Proportionately, without factoring in any independent predictors, it can be stated that about 66% of the total variance is at the individual level, about 9% at the regional level, and about 25% at the country level. The last component is particularly noteworthy and suggests that use of the World Values data and similar surveys in predictive models without special regard for the national contexts is likely to overstate the nature of the socio-demographic relationships in the equations. Much of the explanation is incorporated in the grouping of the data into countries and this context needs to be explicitly tallied in any modeling. Clearly there are large and significant differences in social trust between the countries in the sample. Attention to the regional and country residuals will be given after the multilevel modeling is completed. Though mapping and graphing of the residuals from the variance components model can help in the selection of independent variables, enough is known about the correlates of social trust from the work of Putnam (2000) and Newton (1999; 2001) that we can proceed to the fitting of the models.
Unlike social trust, the variance components model for volunteerism shows no significant coefficient at the regional level (see Table 1) and thus, one can proceed to a model with only two levels, individual and country. Surprisingly, the variance components suggest that the national level is more important than the individual for this factor – stated another way, the country where a person lives is more important in understanding the variation among individuals in volunteerism than the characteristics of the persons surveyed. It is evident from the map (Figure 2) that strong national discrepancies in volunteerism exist due to cultural traditions, political regime character, religious affiliations and the strength of the non-governmental sector. The variance components model confirms this and produces the surprising finding that country-level factors are more significant than personal differences. The intercept (.359) is also significant and when converted from the logit form, shows that the odds of respondents engaging in some volunteerism as .494 (the binary outcome variable measures any active voluntary membership).
Table 1: Variance Components Model for the Trust in
People, Volunteerism and Political Interest
a) Trust
in People
|
Parameter |
Estimate |
Standard
Error |
Fixed
Parameter |
β1jk |
-1.124 |
.089 |
|
|
|
|
Random
Effects Level |
|
|
|
3-
Country |
v1k |
.344 |
.077 |
2-
Region |
u1jk |
.141 |
.019 |
1-
Respondent |
e0ijk |
1.00 |
.000 |
b)
Volunteerism
|
Parameter |
Estimate |
Standard
Error |
Fixed
parameter |
β1jk |
0.359 |
.026 |
|
|
|
|
Random
Effects Level |
|
|
|
3-
Country |
v1k |
.999 |
.091 |
2 –
Region |
u1jk |
.000 |
.000 |
1 –
Respondent |
e0ijk |
.793 |
.020 |
c)
Political Interest
|
Parameter |
Estimate |
Standard
Error |
Fixed
parameter |
β1jk |
-0.032 |
.087 |
|
|
|
|
Random
Effects Level |
|
|
|
3-
Country |
v1k |
0.238 |
.020 |
2 –
Region |
u1jk |
0.137 |
.020 |
1 –
Respondent |
e0ijk |
0.934 |
.010 |
The final variance components model for political interest shows, unsurprisingly, that the odds of political interest are small. When the fixed component (-.032) is converted from its logit form, the proportion of respondents showing political interest is only .225. Since the question was asked in combination with a range of other possibilities in the social and cultural sphere, this low value is not too surprising – and again the map (Figure 3) and summary statistics show a large range in political interest between states. The variances of the random terms are all highly significant and suggest a three level model. Proportionately, 71% of the variance is attributed to the individual level, 10% to the regional level, and the remainder (19%) to the country level. For most democratic and social values, one might expect these sorts of ratios. About two-thirds to three-quarters of the variance is attributed to individuals and the remainder split between the regional and national levels, with the bulk of this remainder associated with the national contexts.
In modeling the variance of the respective dependent variables (social trust, volunteerism and political interest), I opted to use the predictors that had been found to be related significantly to these sorts of political outcomes, as reported in the book edited by Norris (1999b). Given the presence of collinearity, I dropped the predictor with the weakest correlations; in general, I was looking for a model that met the theoretical specifications of the democracy literature, was parsimonious (an especially crucial factor in multilevel modeling with many random terms), met the requirements of the multilevel method and reported only significant coefficients. The results of the final models are presented in Table 2; these models include dummy terms (established and new/non-democracies) for social trust and volunteerism that were included in the model as a result of the patterning in the residuals. The penultimate models did not have these dummy variables.
In the
first model for social trust, left-right self-placement on an ideological scale
has a negative coefficient – those self-identified as leftists are less
trusting (Table 2). Social trust is also
strongly and positively related to life satisfaction, a clear replication of
the
The
results for social trust reported in Table 2 are not surprising given the
extensive previous work on the subject in the
Table 2: Final Multilevel Models for Trust
in People, Volunteerism and Political Interest
a) Trust in People
Variable |
Estimate |
Standard
Error |
Fixed Terms |
|
|
Intercept |
-.565 |
.135 |
Left-right
Self Placement |
-0.014 |
.007 |
Life
Satisfaction |
.040 |
.006 |
Subjective
Social Class |
-0.104 |
.016 |
Societal
Change |
-.118 |
.029 |
Church
Attendance |
-.036 |
.008 |
Est.-New
Democracy |
-.674 |
.183 |
|
|
|
Random Terms |
|
|
3-
Country |
.325 |
.074 |
2-
Region |
.147 |
.020 |
1-
Respondent |
1.000 |
.000 |
b)
Volunteerism
Fixed Terms |
Estimate |
Standard
Error |
Intercept |
.415 |
.128 |
Life
Satisfaction |
.010 |
.006 |
Employment
Status |
.015 |
.006 |
Est.-New
Democracy |
-.537 |
.271 |
|
|
|
Random Terms |
|
|
3-
Country |
.914 |
.177 |
1-
Respondent |
1.00 |
.00 |
c)
Political Interest
Fixed Terms |
Estimate |
Standard
Error |
Intercept |
..244 |
.155 |
Material/Post
Materialist |
.236 |
.026 |
Subjective
Social Class |
-.179 |
.018 |
Gender |
-.436 |
.030 |
Democracy
Indecisive |
.137 |
.019 |
Marital
Status |
-.019 |
.007 |
Age |
.013 |
.001 |
Societal
Change Direction |
-.162 |
.028 |
|
|
|
Random Terms |
|
|
3-
Country |
.245 |
.056 |
2-Region |
.126 |
.019 |
1-
Respondent |
1.000 |
.000 |
In the final model for social
trust, the intercept value (-.565), converted to an odds ratio, yields an
average level of trust by the stereotypical individual of .633. More importantly, there is a large and
significant variation across countries.
Examining the “caterpillar plot” of the residuals from the penultimate
model (same as the final model but missing the democracy dummy variable) shows
the clear trend. In Figure 4, the
confidence interval bands that do not intersect the mean value (horizontal line
at 0.0) show countries where social trust is significantly over – or
under-predicted. Eleven countries show
significant average under-prediction (more trust than would be expected on the
basis of the predictors) whilst another eleven show significant over-prediction
(less trust than would be expected). In
rank-order, the under-predicted values are for
As noted earlier, the final multilevel model for
volunteerism did not require the inclusion of a second-level coefficient for
region and therefore, a two level model (individual and country) is presented
in Table 2. The intercept value (.415)
translates into a volunteering odd-ratio of .534 for individuals (active in any
organization). With the introduction of
the fixed terms in the model, the contribution of the random terms to the
variance is about equal (.914 and 1.0).
But as noted above, there are dramatic country to country differences in
this ratio of volunteerism. Volunteerism
at the individual level was significantly related only to two predictors; those
who have a greater life satisfaction volunteer more, as do those with a higher
employment status. These relationships
are not surprising since it is expected that volunteerism would be higher for
those with the time and the means to take part in such activism. Putnam (2000) has noted this phenomenon in
the
Twenty-five residuals on the over-predicted end of the
rank-order and twenty-two on the under-predicted end of the graph do not have
confidence limits that overlap 0.0. (Figure 5). The wider range of values and the increase
in the number of significant residuals is unsurprising given the nature of the
model - The most over-predicted country is Slovenia, followed in order by
Moldova, Bulgaria, Turkey, Austria, West Germany, France, Taiwan, Georgia,
Latvia, Estonia, Bosnia, Chile, Lithuania, Colombia, Ukraine, Uruguay and
Serbia. At the other end of the
rank-order (volunteerism higher than expected on the basis of the two
predictors) is Argentina, followed by Norway, Mexico, Australia, Brazil,
Hungary, Northern Ireland, Nigeria, Poland, United States, Netherlands,
Macedonia, Ireland, Puerto Rico, Finland, Croatia, South Africa, and
Sweden. The residual pattern for the 57
countries in the analysis, while generally conforming to expectations, offers a
few surprises.
The final multilevel model was also
the most complex with seven independent predictors included in the equation
(Table 2). Many of the relationships are
relatively weak, though significant. The
log-odds ratio of political interest, .415, derived from the transformation of
the intercept value (.244) is small; this may be attributed to the nature of
the question which posed political interest against a range of other interests
of the individuals surveyed. Higher
political interest was expressed by individuals with a more post-materialist
orientation (using the 12 point scale of Inglehart,
2000), by individuals with a (subjective) higher social class, by men, by those
who disagree with the statement that democracy is indecisive, by married
individuals, by older voters, and by those who believe that a change in the
societal direction is needed (Table 2).
There are no surprises in this list. Once again, interest in the
functioning of a democracy is expressed by those with the time, inclination (as
a result of social status) and resources to pay attention to politics. As is clear from surveys in former Communist
countries, political interest is strongly related to personal resources. In
a time of stress, those caught by the changing nature of economic life are
unable to take part.
Unlike the other two models, the
display of the residuals did not help to clarify the regional or country aggregations
of over- and under-prediction (Figure 6).
In contrast to the previous two displays, the confidence intervals are
narrow; fourteen countries have significant positive values (under-prediction)
and seventeen have significant negative values (over-prediction). The rank order (highest
over-prediction) runs from
This multilevel
analysis has indicated a range of geographic effects. In all three models, the national context is
an important presence in determining the outcome of democratic opinions on
social trust, volunteerism and political interest. In two of the three models (for social trust
and political interest), the regional level had a significant presence and
required the estimation of three level models.
Part of the explanation of the varying presence of region as a factor
could be due to the inconsistent definition of region in the World Values
survey. Region size is highly variable
from country to country (ranging from the nine large Census regions in the
The compositional correlates of political attitudes were consistent with previous studies and offered no surprises. What is most impressive from the multilevel model fitting is the residual pattern for countries that seems to have taken on a macro-regional form on a global basis and also one that corresponds to the disparity between old and new democracies. While the pattern in the residuals can be explained by the addition of a dummy variable separating states into these two types, it is entirely possible that further addition of explanatory variables at the country level will assist in accounting for any remaining variance. Since the main purpose of this paper was to establish the nature of the geographic effects in the distribution of democratic values, this further analysis is left for a later paper.
Conclusions
Geographers have insisted for over three decades that the patterning or spatial autocorrelation visible in the distribution of political phenomena cannot be explained away by the distribution of socio-demographic variables or the clustering of individuals of similar socio-demographic character. Controlling for these compositional effects offers one insight into the “geography” that remains but a better alternative is to directly model the spatial element. Multilevel modeling offers a compromise between the usual alternatives, blending individual and aggregate data and allowing a consideration of the multi-scalar effects rather than separate consideration of geographic effects at different scales. Since most social scientific data are hierarchically organized in a nested fashion, the multilevel approach is tailor-made for modeling this type of information.
In the case of democratic values, the scant evidence from social science surveys over the past 3 decades is that a diffusion of belief in democratic principles has spread at the same time as the growth in the ratio of “dissatisfied democrats” has been noted (Norris, 1999c). Public opinion surveys show strong attachment to principles of free expression, civil liberties and political choices in the new democracies, even though many of them have neither little historical memory of such traditions nor much experience of freedom. Efforts of non-governmental agencies based in Western countries to promote grassroots democracy in the form of non-governmental organizations and advocacy groups for women’s rights, the environment, minority and human rights, and protection of constitutional gains have been evident in the new democracies. Though their contact with the citizens of the new democracies is relatively small, their efforts are not going unnoticed by the regimes. By training cadres of educators, it is hoped to spread the notions of Western-style democracies and imbue the newly-democratized societies with the values that have helped to sustain the Western democracies. Continuous pressure from powerful states and the threat to withhold foreign aid to repressive regimes act as powerful incentives for governments and elites at least to feign democratic credentials. While the ratios expressing beliefs in democratic values are still relatively small compared to their Western counterparts, they are nevertheless growing and further diffusion might bridge the gap that is currently evident between the West and the rest. The so-called “democratic deficit” applies not only to the gap between citizens and their governments but also to the disparity between the old and new democracies.
While there seems to be a growing acceptance of the value of democratic governance and the principles that underlie it across the globe, this study has highlighted the country-specific character of democracy. The evidence in the multilevel regression equations is strong and consistent that the attitudes of individuals are conditioned by their location – in regions and countries of specific character. Clear and unambiguous geographic effects in the equations and residuals supports the position of geographers that place matters in the sense that it shapes the local debates and political character and this historical memory remains embedded in the political expressions. This is not to claim that place effects are unchanging and inviolate; rather, geographers hold that places both shape the attitudes and behavior of their residents and in turn, are shaped by the collective expression of this popular will in a reciprocal manner. Countries or nation-states as they are frequently mislabeled are the most powerful territorial expression and their power to shape identity and political behavior remains unparalleled, despite claims of the demise of the state in a globalized world. A full account of citizen preferences, practices and values requires not only knowledge of the compositional characteristics of the individual but also one further characteristic – where she or he lives.
Abramson,
P. and R. Inglehart (1995) Value Change in Global
Perspective.
Agnew, J. A. (1996) “Mapping politics: How context
counts in geography.” Political
Geography 15, 129-146.
Agnew, J.A. (2001) Reinventing Geopolitics: Geographies of Modern Statehood.
Anselin, L. (1988) Spatial Econometrics.
Anselin, L. (1998) Spacestat Version 1.90: User’s Guide.
Anderson, L. (ed) (1999) Transitions
to Democracy.
Bermeo, N.
(1999) Myths of moderation: Confrontation and conflict during democratic
transitions Transitions to Democracy.
Boeninger, E.
(1997)
Bollen, K.
(1993) Liberal democracy: Validity and method factors in cross-national
measures. American Sociological Review 54, 612-21.
Bratton, M. and N. van der Walle
(1997) Democratic Experiments in
Diamond, L. and J. Linz (1989) Introduction. in L. Diamond, J. Linz and S.M. Lipset (eds) Democracy in Developing Countries.
Vol. 4:
Goldstein,
H. (1995) Multilevel Statistical Models. 2nd Edition.
Held, D. (1993) “Democracy:
From city-states to cosmospolitan order. In D. Held (ed) Prospects for Democracy: North, South, East, West.
Hox,
J.J. (1995) Applied Multilevel Analysis.
Huber, E., D. Rueschemeyer and J. Stephens (1999) The
paradoxes of contemporary democracy: Formal, participatory and social
dimensions. In L. Anderson (ed) Transitions to
Democracy.
Inglehart, R. (1997) Modernization
and Post-Modernization.
Inglehart, R. (1999) Modernization erodes respect for authority
but increases support for democracy. In P. Norris (ed)
Critical Citizens: Global Support for Democratic Government.
Inglehart, R.
and M. Carballo (1997) Does Latin America exist? (and is there a Confucian culture?): A global analysis of
cross-cultural differences. PS: Political Science and Politics 30,
34-46.
Inglehart, R. (2000) World
Value Surveys and European Values Surveys, 1981-1984, 1990-1993, and 1995-1997.
[Computer file].ICPSR version.
Jones, K. and C. Duncan (1991) Specifying and Estimating
Multilevel Models for Geographical Research. Geographical Analysis 16, 148-160.
Jones, K. and C. Duncan (1996) People and places: the Mmultilevel Model as a general famework
for the quantitative analysis of geographical data. In P. Longley and M.Batty (ed) Spatial Analysis in a GIS Environment.
Joseph,
R. (1997) Democratization in
King,
G. (1996) Why context should not count. Political Geography 15, 159-164.
King,
G. (1997) A Solution to the
Ecological Inference Problem : Reconstructing
Individual Behavior from Aggregate Data.
Kirby,
A. and M.D. Ward (1987) The spatial analysis of peace
and war. Comparative Political Studies
20, 293-313.
Klingenmann, H.-D. (1999) Mapping political support
in the 1990s: Global trends. In P. Norris (ed) Critical
Citizens: Global Support for Democratic Government.
Kreft,
Lipset, S.M. (1959) Some social
requisites of democracy, economic development and political legitimacy. American
Political Science Review 53, 69-105.
Lipset, S.M. (1994) The social
requisites of democracy revisited. American Sociological Review 59,
1-22.
Lipset, S.M., K.R. Seong and J.C.Torres (1993) A
comparative analysis of the social requisites of democracy. Studies in International
Comparative Development 16: 155-75.
MacAllister,
Mishler, W. and R. Rose (2001) What are
the origins of political trust? Testing institutional and
cultural theories in post-Communist societies. Comparative Political Studies 34, 30-62.
Nye,
J.S., P.D. Zellikov and D.C. King (eds) (1997) Why People Don’t
Trust Government.
Norris,
P. (1999a) Conclusions: The growth of critical citizens and its consequences.
In Norris, P. (ed) (1999) Critical Citizens:
Support for Democratic Government.
Norris,
P. (ed) (1999b) Critical Citizens: Support for
Democratic Government.
Norris,
P. (1999c) Introduction: The growth of critical citizens? In Norris, P. (ed) (1999) Critical Citizens: Support for Democratic
Government.
O’Loughlin,
J. (1986) Spatial models of international conflict: Extending theories of war
behavior. Annals, Association of American
Geographers 76, 63-80.
O’Loughlin,
J. (2000) Geography as space and geography as place: The divide between
political science and political geography continues. Geopolitics
5 (3), 126-137.
O’Loughlin,
J. (2001) Geography and democracy: The spatial diffusion of political and civil
rights. In G.-J. Dijkink and
H. Knippenberg (eds)
The Territorial Factor: Political
Geography in a Globalising World.
O’Loughlin, J.
(2002) Spatial and quantitative models in political geography.
In J. Agnew, G. Ó Tuathail and K. Mitchell (eds) A Reader
in Political Geography.
O’Loughlin,
J. and L. Anselin (1991) Bringing geography back to the study of international
relations: Spatial dependence and regional context in
O’Loughlin,
J. and J.E. Bell (2000) The political geography of
civic engagement in
O’Loughlin,
J. , M.D. Ward et
al. (1998) The diffusion of democracy 1946-1994. Annals of the
Association of American Geographers 88, 545-574.
Pattie,
C. and R.J. Johnston (2000) ‘People who talk together vote together’: An
exploration of contextual effects in
Putnam,
R. (1993) Making Democracy Work: Civic
Traditions in Modern
Putnam, R. (2000) Bowling
Alone: The Collapse and Revival of
American Community.
Rasbash, J.
(2000) A User’s Guide to MlwiN.
Rueschemeyer, D., E.H. Stephens and J.D. Stephens (1992) Capitalist Development and Democracy.
Rustow, D.A. (1970) Transitions to
democracy: Toward a dynamic model. Comparative Politics 2, 337-63.
Shin,
M. (2001) The politicization of place in
Tarrow, S. (1996) Making social
science work across space and time: A critical reflection on Robert Putnam’s Making
Democracy Work. American Political Science Review
90, 389-97.
Figure 1: Global Distribution of Trust of Fellow Citizens in the
1990s. Data are from the World
Values survey questions. The value for
each country is computed on a 5-point scale from responses to the question “Generally speaking, would you say that most
people can be trusted or that you can't be too careful in dealing with people?”
Figure 2: Volunteerism in Non-Governmental Organizations in the
1990s. The World Values question
posed for involvement in non-governmental organizations was: “Now I am going to
read off a list of voluntary organizations; for each one, could you tell me
whether you are an active member, an inactive member or not a member of that
type of organization?”.
Figure 3: Political Interest in
the 1990s. The index is derived from the World Value
survey answers to the question: “Please
say, for each of the following, how important is it in your life” Options for
responses were very important, rather important, not very important, not at all
important, and don’t know. Among the
list of interests was “politics”.
Figure 4: Residuals from the
Final Model for Social Trust (three level model with 5 predictors).
Figure 5: Residuals from the
Two-Level Model of Volunteerism
Figure 6: Residuals from the 2 level model of political interest
[1] This research was supported by a grant from
the National Science Foundation and was conducted in the context of the
“Globalization and Democracy” graduate training program in the